Author

Phillip Alday, Douglas Bates, and Reinhold Kliegl

Published

2024-06-27

1 A large-scale designed experiment

Load the packages to be used.

Code
using AlgebraOfGraphics
using Arrow
using CairoMakie
using Chain
using DataFrameMacros
using DataFrames
using Effects
using MixedModels
using MixedModelsMakie
using SMLP2023: dataset
using StandardizedPredictors
using StatsBase

CairoMakie.activate!(; type="svg")
import ProgressMeter
ProgressMeter.ijulia_behavior(:clear)

The English Lexicon Project (Balota et al., 2007) was a large-scale multicenter study to examine properties of English words. It incorporated both a lexical decision task and a word recognition task. Different groups of subjects participated in the different tasks.

2 Extracting data tables from the raw data

The raw data are available as an OSF project as Zip files for each of the tasks. These Zip files contain one data file for each participant, which has a mixture of demographic data, responses on some pre-tests, and the actual trial runs.

Parsing these data files is not fun – see this repository for some of the code used to untangle the data. (This repository is an unregistered Julia package.)

Some lessons from this:

  • When an identifier is described as a “unique subject id”, it probably isn’t.
  • In a multi-center trial, the coordinating center should assign the range of id’s for each satellite site. Failure of a satellite site to stay within its range should result in banishment to a Siberian work camp.
  • As with all data cleaning, the prevailing attitude should be “trust, but verify”. Just because you are told that the file is in a certain format, doesn’t mean it is. Just because you are told that the identifiers are unique doesn’t mean they are, etc.
  • It works best if each file has a well-defined, preferably simple, structure. These data files had two different formats mushed together.
  • This is the idea of “tidy data” - each file contains only one type of record along with well-defined rules of how you relate one file to another.
  • If one of the fields is a date, declare the only acceptable form of writing a date, preferably yyyy-mm-dd. Anyone getting creative about the format of the dates will be required to write the software to parse that form (and that is usually not an easy task).
  • As you make changes in a file, document them. If you look at the EnglishLexicon.jl repository you will see that it is in the form of scripts that take the original Zip files and produce the Arrow files. That way, if necessary, the changes can be undone or modified.
  • Remember, the data are only as useful as their provenance. If you invested a lot of time and money in gathering the data you should treat it as a valued resource and exercise great care with it.
  • The Arrow.jl package allows you to add metadata as key/value pairs, called a Dict (or dictionary). Use this capability. The name of the file is not a suitable location for metadata.

2.1 Trial-level data from the LDT

In the lexical decision task the study participant is shown a character string, under carefully controlled conditions, and responds according to whether they identify the string as a word or not. Two responses are recorded: whether the choice of word/non-word is correct and the time that elapsed between exposure to the string and registering a decision.

Several covariates, some relating to the subject and some relating to the target, were recorded. Initially we consider only the trial-level data.

ldttrial = dataset(:ELP_ldt_trial)
Arrow.Table with 2745952 rows, 5 columns, and schema:
 :subj  Int16
 :seq   Int16
 :acc   Union{Missing, Bool}
 :rt    Int16
 :item  String

with metadata given by a Base.ImmutableDict{String, String} with 3 entries:
  "title"     => "Trial-level data from Lexical Discrimination Task in the Engl…
  "reference" => "Balota et al. (2007), The English Lexicon Project, Behavior R…
  "source"    => "https://osf.io/n63s2"

The two response variables are acc - the accuracy of the response - and rt, the response time in milliseconds. There is one trial-level covariate, seq, the sequence number of the trial within subj. Each subject participated in two sessions on different days, with 2000 trials recorded on the first day.

Notice the metadata with a citation and a URL for the OSF project.

We convert to a DataFrame and add a Boolean column s2 which is true for trials in the second session.

ldttrial = @transform(DataFrame(ldttrial), :s2 = :seq > 2000)
describe(ldttrial)
6×7 DataFrame
Row variable mean min median max nmissing eltype
Symbol Union… Any Union… Any Int64 Type
1 subj 409.31 1 409.0 816 0 Int16
2 seq 1687.21 1 1687.0 3374 0 Int16
3 acc 0.85604 false 1.0 true 1370 Union{Missing, Bool}
4 rt 846.325 -16160 732.0 32061 0 Int16
5 item Aarod zuss 0 String
6 s2 0.407128 false 0.0 true 0 Bool

2.2 Initial data exploration

From the basic summary of ldttrial we can see that there are some questionable response times — negative values and values over 32 seconds.

Because of obvious outliers we will use the median response time, which is not strongly influenced by outliers, rather than the mean response time when summarizing by item or by subject.

Also, there are missing values of the accuracy. We should check if these are associated with particular subjects or particular items.

2.2.1 Summaries by item

To summarize by item we group the trials by item and use combine to produce the various summary statistics. As we will create similar summaries by subject, we incorporate an ‘i’ in the names of these summaries (and an ‘s’ in the name of the summaries by subject) to be able to identify the grouping used.

byitem = @chain ldttrial begin
  groupby(:item)
  @combine(
    :ni = length(:acc),               # no. of obs
    :imiss = count(ismissing, :acc),  # no. of missing acc
    :iacc = count(skipmissing(:acc)), # no. of accurate
    :imedianrt = median(:rt),
  )
  @transform!(
    :wrdlen = Int8(length(:item)),
    :ipropacc = :iacc / :ni
  )
end
80962×7 DataFrame
80937 rows omitted
Row item ni imiss iacc imedianrt wrdlen ipropacc
String Int64 Int64 Int64 Float64 Int8 Float64
1 a 35 0 26 743.0 1 0.742857
2 e 35 0 19 824.0 1 0.542857
3 aah 34 0 21 770.5 3 0.617647
4 aal 34 0 32 702.5 3 0.941176
5 Aaron 33 0 31 625.0 5 0.939394
6 Aarod 33 0 23 810.0 5 0.69697
7 aback 34 0 15 710.0 5 0.441176
8 ahack 34 0 34 662.0 5 1.0
9 abacus 34 0 17 671.5 6 0.5
10 alacus 34 0 29 640.0 6 0.852941
11 abandon 34 0 32 641.0 7 0.941176
12 acandon 34 0 33 725.5 7 0.970588
13 abandoned 34 0 31 667.5 9 0.911765
80951 zoology 33 0 32 623.0 7 0.969697
80952 poology 33 0 32 757.0 7 0.969697
80953 zoom 35 0 34 548.0 4 0.971429
80954 zool 35 0 30 633.0 4 0.857143
80955 zooming 33 0 29 617.0 7 0.878788
80956 sooming 33 0 30 721.0 7 0.909091
80957 zooms 33 0 30 598.0 5 0.909091
80958 cooms 33 0 31 660.0 5 0.939394
80959 zucchini 34 0 29 781.5 8 0.852941
80960 hucchini 34 0 32 727.5 8 0.941176
80961 Zurich 34 0 21 731.5 6 0.617647
80962 Zurach 34 0 26 811.0 6 0.764706

It can be seen that the items occur in word/nonword pairs and the pairs are sorted alphabetically by the word in the pair (ignoring case). We can add the word/nonword status for the items as

byitem.isword = isodd.(eachindex(byitem.item))
describe(byitem)
8×7 DataFrame
Row variable mean min median max nmissing eltype
Symbol Union… Any Union… Any Int64 DataType
1 item Aarod zuss 0 String
2 ni 33.9166 30 34.0 37 0 Int64
3 imiss 0.0169215 0 0.0 2 0 Int64
4 iacc 29.0194 0 31.0 37 0 Int64
5 imedianrt 753.069 458.0 737.5 1691.0 0 Float64
6 wrdlen 7.9988 1 8.0 21 0 Int8
7 ipropacc 0.855616 0.0 0.911765 1.0 0 Float64
8 isword 0.5 false 0.5 true 0 Bool

This table shows that some of the items were never identified correctly. These are

filter(:iacc => iszero, byitem)
9×8 DataFrame
Row item ni imiss iacc imedianrt wrdlen ipropacc isword
String Int64 Int64 Int64 Float64 Int8 Float64 Bool
1 baobab 34 0 0 616.5 6 0.0 true
2 haulage 34 0 0 708.5 7 0.0 true
3 leitmotif 35 0 0 688.0 9 0.0 true
4 miasmal 35 0 0 774.0 7 0.0 true
5 peahen 34 0 0 684.0 6 0.0 true
6 plosive 34 0 0 663.0 7 0.0 true
7 plugugly 33 0 0 709.0 8 0.0 true
8 poshest 34 0 0 740.0 7 0.0 true
9 servo 33 0 0 697.0 5 0.0 true

Notice that these are all words but somewhat obscure words such that none of the subjects exposed to the word identified it correctly.

We can incorporate characteristics like wrdlen and isword back into the original trial table with a “left join”. This operation joins two tables by values in a common column. It is called a left join because the left (or first) table takes precedence, in the sense that every row in the left table is present in the result. If there is no matching row in the second table then missing values are inserted for the columns from the right table in the result.

describe(
  leftjoin!(
    ldttrial,
    select(byitem, :item, :wrdlen, :isword);
    on=:item,
  ),
)
8×7 DataFrame
Row variable mean min median max nmissing eltype
Symbol Union… Any Union… Any Int64 Type
1 subj 409.31 1 409.0 816 0 Int16
2 seq 1687.21 1 1687.0 3374 0 Int16
3 acc 0.85604 false 1.0 true 1370 Union{Missing, Bool}
4 rt 846.325 -16160 732.0 32061 0 Int16
5 item Aarod zuss 0 String
6 s2 0.407128 false 0.0 true 0 Bool
7 wrdlen 7.99835 1 8.0 21 0 Union{Missing, Int8}
8 isword 0.499995 false 0.0 true 0 Union{Missing, Bool}

Notice that the wrdlen and isword variables in this table allow for missing values, because they are derived from the second argument, but there are no missing values for these variables. If there is no need to allow for missing values, there is a slight advantage in disallowing them in the element type, because the code to check for and handle missing values is not needed.

This could be done separately for each column or for the whole data frame, as in

describe(disallowmissing!(ldttrial; error=false))
8×7 DataFrame
Row variable mean min median max nmissing eltype
Symbol Union… Any Union… Any Int64 Type
1 subj 409.31 1 409.0 816 0 Int16
2 seq 1687.21 1 1687.0 3374 0 Int16
3 acc 0.85604 false 1.0 true 1370 Union{Missing, Bool}
4 rt 846.325 -16160 732.0 32061 0 Int16
5 item Aarod zuss 0 String
6 s2 0.407128 false 0.0 true 0 Bool
7 wrdlen 7.99835 1 8.0 21 0 Int8
8 isword 0.499995 false 0.0 true 0 Bool

The named argument error=false is required because there is one column, acc, that does incorporate missing values. If error=false were not given then the error thrown when trying to disallowmissing on the acc column would be propagated and the top-level call would fail.

A barchart of the word length counts, Figure 1, shows that the majority of the items are between 3 and 14 characters.

Code
let
  wlen = 1:21
  draw(
    data((; wrdlen=wlen, count=counts(byitem.wrdlen, wlen))) *
    mapping(:wrdlen => "Length of word", :count) *
    visual(BarPlot),
  )
end
Figure 1: Barchart of word lengths in the items used in the lexical decision task.

To examine trends in accuracy by word length we plot the proportion accurate versus word-length separately for words and non-words with the area of each marker proportional to the number of observations for that combination (Figure 2).

Code
let
  itemsummry = combine(
    groupby(byitem, [:wrdlen, :isword]),
    :ni => sum,
    :imiss => sum,
    :iacc => sum,
  )
  @transform!(
    itemsummry,
    :iacc_mean = :iacc_sum / (:ni_sum - :imiss_sum)
  )
  @transform!(itemsummry, :msz = sqrt((:ni_sum - :imiss_sum) / 800))
  draw(
    data(itemsummry) * mapping(
      :wrdlen => "Word length",
      :iacc_mean => "Proportion accurate";
      color=:isword,
      markersize=:msz,
    );
    figure=(; resolution=(800, 450)),
  )
end
Figure 2: Proportion of accurate trials in the LDT versus word length separately for words and non-words. The area of the marker is proportional to the number of observations represented.

The pattern in the range of word lengths with non-negligible counts (there are points in the plot down to word lengths of 1 and up to word lengths of 21 but these points are very small) is that the accuracy for words is nearly constant at about 84% and the accuracy for nonwords is slightly higher until lengths of 13, at which point it falls off a bit.

2.2.2 Summaries by subject

A summary of accuracy and median response time by subject

bysubj = @chain ldttrial begin
  groupby(:subj)
  @combine(
    :ns = length(:acc),               # no. of obs
    :smiss = count(ismissing, :acc),  # no. of missing acc
    :sacc = count(skipmissing(:acc)), # no. of accurate
    :smedianrt = median(:rt),
  )
  @transform!(:spropacc = :sacc / :ns)
end
814×6 DataFrame
789 rows omitted
Row subj ns smiss sacc smedianrt spropacc
Int16 Int64 Int64 Int64 Float64 Float64
1 1 3374 0 3158 554.0 0.935981
2 2 3372 1 3031 960.0 0.898873
3 3 3372 3 3006 813.0 0.891459
4 4 3374 1 3062 619.0 0.907528
5 5 3374 0 2574 677.0 0.762893
6 6 3374 0 2927 855.0 0.867516
7 7 3374 4 2877 918.5 0.852697
8 8 3372 1 2731 1310.0 0.809905
9 9 3374 13 2669 657.0 0.791049
10 10 3374 0 2722 757.0 0.806758
11 11 3374 0 2894 632.0 0.857736
12 12 3374 4 2979 692.0 0.882928
13 13 3374 2 2980 1114.0 0.883225
803 805 3374 5 2881 534.0 0.853883
804 806 3374 1 3097 841.5 0.917902
805 807 3374 3 2994 704.0 0.887374
806 808 3374 2 2751 630.5 0.815353
807 809 3372 4 2603 627.0 0.771945
808 810 3374 1 3242 603.5 0.960877
809 811 3374 2 2861 827.0 0.847955
810 812 3372 6 3012 471.0 0.893238
811 813 3372 4 2932 823.0 0.869514
812 814 3374 1 3070 773.0 0.909899
813 815 3374 1 3024 602.0 0.896266
814 816 3374 0 2950 733.0 0.874333

shows some anomalies

describe(bysubj)
6×7 DataFrame
Row variable mean min median max nmissing eltype
Symbol Float64 Real Float64 Real Int64 DataType
1 subj 409.311 1 409.5 816 0 Int16
2 ns 3373.41 3370 3374.0 3374 0 Int64
3 smiss 1.68305 0 1.0 22 0 Int64
4 sacc 2886.33 1727 2928.0 3286 0 Int64
5 smedianrt 760.992 205.0 735.0 1804.0 0 Float64
6 spropacc 0.855613 0.511855 0.868031 0.973918 0 Float64

First, some subjects are accurate on only about half of their trials, which is the proportion that would be expected from random guessing. A plot of the median response time versus proportion accurate, Figure 3, shows that the subjects with lower accuracy are some of the fastest responders, further indicating that these subjects are sacrificing accuracy for speed.

Code
draw(
  data(bysubj) *
  mapping(
    :spropacc => "Proportion accurate",
    :smedianrt => "Median response time (ms)",
  ) *
  (visual(Scatter) + smooth())
)
Figure 3: Median response time versus proportion accurate by subject in the LDT.

As described in Balota et al. (2007), the participants performed the trials in blocks of 250 followed by a short break. During the break they were given feedback concerning accuracy and response latency in the previous block of trials. If the accuracy was less than 80% the participant was encouraged to improve their accuracy. Similarly, if the mean response latency was greater than 1000 ms, the participant was encouraged to decrease their response time. During the trials immediate feedback was given if the response was incorrect.

Nevertheless, approximately 15% of the subjects were unable to maintain 80% accuracy on their trials

count(<(0.8), bysubj.spropacc) / nrow(bysubj)
0.15233415233415235

and there is some association of faster response times with low accuracy. The majority of the subjects whose median response time is less than 500 ms. are accurate on less than 75% of their trials. Another way of characterizing the relationship is that none of the subjects with 90% accuracy or greater had a median response time less than 500 ms.

minimum(@subset(bysubj, :spropacc > 0.9).smedianrt)
505.0

It is common in analyses of response latency in a lexical discrimination task to consider only the latencies on correct identifications and to trim outliers. In Balota et al. (2007) a two-stage outlier removal strategy was used; first removing responses less than 200 ms or greater than 3000 ms then removing responses more than three standard deviations from the participant’s mean response.

As described in Section 2.2.3 we will analyze these data on a speed scale (the inverse of response time) using only the first-stage outlier removal of response latencies less than 200 ms or greater than 3000 ms. On the speed scale the limits are 0.333 per second up to 5 per second.

To examine the effects of the fast but inaccurate responders we will fit models to the data from all the participants and to the data from the 85% of participants who maintained an overall accuracy of 80% or greater.

pruned = @chain ldttrial begin
  @subset(!ismissing(:acc), 200  :rt  3000,)
  leftjoin!(select(bysubj, :subj, :spropacc); on=:subj)
  dropmissing!
end
size(pruned)
(2714311, 9)
describe(pruned)
9×7 DataFrame
Row variable mean min median max nmissing eltype
Symbol Union… Any Union… Any Int64 DataType
1 subj 409.802 1 410.0 816 0 Int16
2 seq 1684.56 1 1684.0 3374 0 Int16
3 acc 0.859884 false 1.0 true 0 Bool
4 rt 838.712 200 733.0 3000 0 Int16
5 item Aarod zuss 0 String
6 s2 0.40663 false 0.0 true 0 Bool
7 wrdlen 7.99244 1 8.0 21 0 Int8
8 isword 0.500126 false 1.0 true 0 Bool
9 spropacc 0.857169 0.511855 0.869295 0.973918 0 Float64

2.2.3 Choice of response scale

As we have indicated, generally the response times are analyzed for the correct identifications only. Furthermore, unrealistically large or small response times are eliminated. For this example we only use the responses between 200 and 3000 ms.

A density plot of the pruned response times, Figure 4, shows they are skewed to the right.

Code
draw(
  data(pruned) *
  mapping(:rt => "Response time (ms.) for correct responses") *
  AlgebraOfGraphics.density();
  figure=(; resolution=(800, 450)),
)
Figure 4: Kernel density plot of the pruned response times (ms.) in the LDT.

In such cases it is common to transform the response to a scale such as the logarithm of the response time or to the speed of the response, which is the inverse of the response time.

The density of the response speed, in responses per second, is shown in Figure 5.

Code
draw(
  data(pruned) *
  mapping(
    :rt => (x -> 1000 / x) => "Response speed (s⁻¹) for correct responses") *
  AlgebraOfGraphics.density();
  figure=(; resolution=(800, 450)),
)
Figure 5: Kernel density plot of the pruned response speed in the LDT.

Figure 4 and Figure 5 indicate that it may be more reasonable to establish a lower bound of 1/3 second (333 ms) on the response latency, corresponding to an upper bound of 3 per second on the response speed. However, only about one half of one percent of the correct responses have latencies in the range of 200 ms. to 333 ms.

count(
  r -> !ismissing(r.acc) && 200 < r.rt < 333,
  eachrow(ldttrial),
) / count(!ismissing, ldttrial.acc)
0.005867195806137328

so the exact position of the lower cut-off point on the response latencies is unlikely to be very important.

If you examine the code for (fit-elpldtspeeddens?), you will see that the conversion from rt to speed is done inline rather than creating and storing a new variable in the DataFrame.

I prefer to keep the DataFrame simple with the integer variables (e.g. :rt) if possible.

I recommend using the StandardizedPredictors.jl capabilities to center numeric variables or convert to zscores.

2.2.4 Transformation of response and the form of the model

As noted in Box & Cox (1964), a transformation of the response that produces a more Gaussian distribution often will also produce a simpler model structure. For example, Figure 6 shows the smoothed relationship between word length and response time for words and non-words separately,

Code
draw(
  data(pruned) *
  mapping(
    :wrdlen => "Word length",
    :rt => "Response time (ms)";
    :color => :isword,
  ) * smooth()
)
Figure 6: Scatterplot smooths of response time versus word length in the LDT.

and Figure 7 shows the similar relationships for speed

Code
draw(
  data(pruned) *
  mapping(
    :wrdlen => "Word length",
    :rt => (x -> 1000/x) => "Speed of response (s⁻¹)";
    :color => :isword,
  ) * smooth()
)
Figure 7: Scatterplot smooths of response speed versus word length in the LDT.

For the most part the smoother lines in Figure 7 are reasonably straight. The small amount of curvature is associated with short word lengths, say less than 4 characters, of which there are comparatively few in the study.

Figure 8 shows a “violin plot” - the empirical density of the response speed by word length separately for words and nonwords. The lines on the plot are fit by linear regression.

Code
let
  plt = data(@subset(pruned, :wrdlen > 3, :wrdlen < 14))
  plt *= mapping(
    :wrdlen => "Word length",
    :rt => (x -> 1000/x) => "Speed of response (s⁻¹)",
    color=:isword,
    side=:isword,
  )
  plt *= visual(Violin)
  draw(plt, axis=(; limits=(nothing, (0.0, 2.8))))
end
Figure 8: Empirical density of response speed versus word length by word/non-word status.

2.3 Models with scalar random effects

A major purpose of the English Lexicon Project is to characterize the items (words or nonwords) according to the observed accuracy of identification and to response latency, taking into account subject-to-subject variability, and to relate these to lexical characteristics of the items.

In Balota et al. (2007) the item response latency is characterized by the average response latency from the correct trials after outlier removal.

Mixed-effects models allow us greater flexibility and, we hope, precision in characterizing the items by controlling for subject-to-subject variability and for item characteristics such as word/nonword and item length.

We begin with a model that has scalar random effects for item and for subject and incorporates fixed-effects for word/nonword and for item length and for the interaction of these terms.

2.3.1 Establish the contrasts

Because there are a large number of items in the data set it is important to assign a Grouping() contrast to item (and, less importantly, to subj). For the isword factor we will use an EffectsCoding contrast with the base level as false. The non-words are assigned -1 in this contrast and the words are assigned +1. The wrdlen covariate is on its original scale but centered at 8 characters.

Thus the (Intercept) coefficient is the predicted speed of response for a typical subject and typical item (without regard to word/non-word status) of 8 characters.

Set these contrasts

contrasts = Dict(
  :subj => Grouping(),
  :item => Grouping(),
  :isword => EffectsCoding(; base=false),
  :wrdlen => Center(8),
)
Dict{Symbol, Any} with 4 entries:
  :item   => Grouping()
  :wrdlen => Center(8)
  :isword => EffectsCoding(false, nothing)
  :subj   => Grouping()

and fit a first model with simple, scalar, random effects for subj and item.

elm01 = let
  form = @formula(
    1000 / rt ~ 1 + isword * wrdlen + (1 | item) + (1 | subj)
  )
  fit(MixedModel, form, pruned; contrasts)
end
Minimizing 53    Time: 0:00:06 ( 0.12  s/it)
Est. SE z p σ_item σ_subj
(Intercept) 1.3758 0.0090 153.69 <1e-99 0.1185 0.2550
isword: true 0.0625 0.0005 131.35 <1e-99
wrdlen(centered: 8) -0.0436 0.0002 -225.38 <1e-99
isword: true & wrdlen(centered: 8) -0.0056 0.0002 -28.83 <1e-99
Residual 0.3781

The predicted response speed by word length and word/nonword status can be summarized as

effects(Dict(:isword => [false, true], :wrdlen => 4:2:12), elm01)
10×6 DataFrame
Row wrdlen isword 1000 / rt err lower upper
Int64 Bool Float64 Float64 Float64 Float64
1 4 false 1.46555 0.00903111 1.45652 1.47458
2 6 false 1.38947 0.00898124 1.38049 1.39845
3 8 false 1.31338 0.00896459 1.30442 1.32235
4 10 false 1.2373 0.00898134 1.22832 1.24628
5 12 false 1.16121 0.00903129 1.15218 1.17025
6 4 true 1.6351 0.0090311 1.62607 1.64413
7 6 true 1.5367 0.00898124 1.52772 1.54569
8 8 true 1.43831 0.00896459 1.42934 1.44727
9 10 true 1.33991 0.00898133 1.33092 1.34889
10 12 true 1.24151 0.00903128 1.23248 1.25054

If we restrict to only those subjects with 80% accuracy or greater the model becomes

elm02 = let
  form = @formula(
    1000 / rt ~ 1 + isword * wrdlen + (1 | item) + (1 | subj)
  )
  dat = @subset(pruned, :spropacc > 0.8)
  fit(MixedModel, form, dat; contrasts)
end
Minimizing 54    Time: 0:00:04 (78.69 ms/it)
Est. SE z p σ_item σ_subj
(Intercept) 1.3611 0.0088 153.98 <1e-99 0.1247 0.2318
isword: true 0.0656 0.0005 133.73 <1e-99
wrdlen(centered: 8) -0.0444 0.0002 -222.65 <1e-99
isword: true & wrdlen(centered: 8) -0.0057 0.0002 -28.73 <1e-99
Residual 0.3342
effects(Dict(:isword => [false, true], :wrdlen => 4:2:12), elm02)
10×6 DataFrame
Row wrdlen isword 1000 / rt err lower upper
Int64 Bool Float64 Float64 Float64 Float64
1 4 false 1.45036 0.00892461 1.44144 1.45929
2 6 false 1.37297 0.00887095 1.3641 1.38184
3 8 false 1.29557 0.00885302 1.28672 1.30443
4 10 false 1.21818 0.00887105 1.20931 1.22705
5 12 false 1.14078 0.0089248 1.13186 1.14971
6 4 true 1.62735 0.0089246 1.61842 1.63627
7 6 true 1.52702 0.00887095 1.51815 1.53589
8 8 true 1.4267 0.00885302 1.41784 1.43555
9 10 true 1.32637 0.00887104 1.3175 1.33524
10 12 true 1.22605 0.00892478 1.21712 1.23497

The differences in the fixed-effects parameter estimates between a model fit to the full data set and one fit to the data from accurate responders only, are small.

However, the random effects for the item, while highly correlated, are not perfectly correlated.

Code
CairoMakie.activate!(; type="png")
disallowmissing!(
  leftjoin!(
    byitem,
    leftjoin!(
      rename!(DataFrame(raneftables(elm01)[:item]), [:item, :elm01]),
      rename!(DataFrame(raneftables(elm02)[:item]), [:item, :elm02]);
      on=:item,
    ),
    on=:item,
  ),
)
disallowmissing!(
  leftjoin!(
    bysubj,
    leftjoin!(
      rename!(DataFrame(raneftables(elm01)[:subj]), [:subj, :elm01]),
      rename!(DataFrame(raneftables(elm02)[:subj]), [:subj, :elm02]);
      on=:subj,
    ),
    on=:subj,
  ); error=false,
)
draw(
  data(byitem) * mapping(
    :elm01 => "Conditional means of item random effects for model elm01",
    :elm02 => "Conditional means of item random effects for model elm02";
    color=:isword,
  ) * visual(Scatter; alpha=0.2);
  axis=(; width=600, height=600),
)
Figure 9: Conditional means of scalar random effects for item in model elm01, fit to the pruned data, versus those for model elm02, fit to the pruned data with inaccurate subjects removed.
Note

Adjust the alpha on Figure 9.

Figure 9 is exactly of the form that would be expected in a sample from a correlated multivariate Gaussian distribution. The correlation of the two sets of conditional means is about 96%.

cor(Matrix(select(byitem, :elm01, :elm02)))
2×2 Matrix{Float64}:
 1.0       0.958655
 0.958655  1.0

These models take only a few seconds to fit on a modern laptop computer, which is quite remarkable given the size of the data set and the number of random effects.

The amount of time to fit more complex models will be much greater so we may want to move those fits to more powerful server computers. We can split the tasks of fitting and analyzing a model between computers by saving the optimization summary after the model fit and later creating the MixedModel object followed by restoring the optsum object.

saveoptsum("./fits/elm01.json", elm01);
elm01a = restoreoptsum!(
  let
    form = @formula(
      1000 / rt ~ 1 + isword * wrdlen + (1 | item) + (1 | subj)
    )
    MixedModel(form, pruned; contrasts)
  end,
  "./fits/elm01.json",
)
Est. SE z p σ_item σ_subj
(Intercept) 1.3758 0.0090 153.69 <1e-99 0.1185 0.2550
isword: true 0.0625 0.0005 131.35 <1e-99
wrdlen(centered: 8) -0.0436 0.0002 -225.38 <1e-99
isword: true & wrdlen(centered: 8) -0.0056 0.0002 -28.83 <1e-99
Residual 0.3781

Other covariates associated with the item are available as

elpldtitem = DataFrame(dataset("ELP_ldt_item"))
describe(elpldtitem)
9×7 DataFrame
Row variable mean min median max nmissing eltype
Symbol Union… Any Union… Any Int64 Type
1 item Aarod zuss 0 String
2 Ortho_N 1.53309 0 1.0 25 0 Int8
3 BG_Sum 13938.4 11 13026.0 59803 177 Union{Missing, Int32}
4 BG_Mean 1921.25 5.5 1907.0 6910.0 177 Union{Missing, Float32}
5 BG_Freq_By_Pos 2043.08 0 1928.0 6985 4 Union{Missing, Int16}
6 itemno 40481.5 1 40481.5 80962 0 Int32
7 isword 0.5 false 0.5 true 0 Bool
8 wrdlen 7.9988 1 8.0 21 0 Int8
9 pairno 20241.0 1 20241.0 40481 0 Int32

and those associated with the subject are

elpldtsubj = DataFrame(dataset("ELP_ldt_subj"))
describe(elpldtsubj)
20×7 DataFrame
Row variable mean min median max nmissing eltype
Symbol Union… Any Any Any Int64 Type
1 subj 409.311 1 409.5 816 0 Int16
2 univ Kansas Wayne State 0 String
3 sex f m 8 Union{Missing, String}
4 DOB 1938-06-07 1984-11-14 0 Date
5 MEQ 44.4932 19.0 44.0 75.0 8 Union{Missing, Float32}
6 vision 5.51169 0 6.0 7 1 Union{Missing, Int8}
7 hearing 5.86101 0 6.0 7 1 Union{Missing, Int8}
8 educatn 8.89681 1 12.0 28 0 Int8
9 ncorrct 29.8505 5 30.0 40 18 Union{Missing, Int8}
10 rawscor 31.9925 13 32.0 40 18 Union{Missing, Int8}
11 vocabAge 17.8123 10.3 17.8 21.0 19 Union{Missing, Float32}
12 shipTime 3.0861 0 3.0 9 1 Union{Missing, Int8}
13 readTime 2.50215 0.0 2.0 15.0 1 Union{Missing, Float32}
14 preshlth 5.48708 0 6.0 7 1 Union{Missing, Int8}
15 pasthlth 4.92989 0 5.0 7 1 Union{Missing, Int8}
16 S1start 2001-03-16T13:49:27 2001-10-16T11:38:28.500 2003-07-29T18:48:44 0 DateTime
17 S2start 2001-03-19T10:00:35 2001-10-19T14:24:19.500 2003-07-30T13:07:45 0 DateTime
18 MEQstrt 2001-03-22T18:32:00 2001-10-23T11:26:13 2003-07-30T14:30:49 7 Union{Missing, DateTime}
19 filename 101DATA.LDT Data998.LDT 0 String
20 frstLang English other 8 Union{Missing, String}

For the simple model elm01 the estimated standard deviation of the random effects for subject is greater than that of the random effects for item, a common occurrence. A caterpillar plot, Figure 10,

Code
qqcaterpillar!(
  Figure(resolution=(800, 650)),
  ranefinfo(elm01, :subj),
)
Figure 10: Conditional means and 95% prediction intervals for subject random effects in elm01.

shows definite distinctions between subjects because the widths of the prediction intervals are small compared to the range of the conditional modes. Also, there is at least one outlier with a conditional mode over 1.0.

Figure 11 is the corresponding caterpillar plot for model elm02 fit to the data with inaccurate responders eliminated.

Code
qqcaterpillar!(
  Figure(resolution=(800, 650)),
  ranefinfo(elm02, :subj),
)
Figure 11: Conditional means and 95% prediction intervals for subject random effects in elm02.
Back to top

References

Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Cortese, M. J., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., Neely, J. H., Nelson, D. L., Simpson, G. B., & Treiman, R. (2007). The english lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193014
Box, G. E. P., & Cox, D. R. (1964). An analysis of transformations. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 26(2), 211–243. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1964.tb00553.x